What's the difference between hypermetric and syllable?

Hypermetric


Definition:

Example Sentences:

  • (1) These saccades were hypermetric and were followed immediately, without any intersaccadic interval, by a large, oppositely directed saccade (dynamic overshoot).
  • (2) Photoelectric eye movement recording in 9 patients with cerebellar disorders defined three features of saccadic overshoot dysmetria: (i) saccades were hypermetric and successively diminished in amplitude; (ii) saccadic initiation interval averaged 173 ms: and (iii) eye position was constant during the intersaccadic period.
  • (3) to the lesion side) tended to be hypermetric and saccades made in response to a contralateral target step were strongly hypometric.
  • (4) For diagnosis of myasthenic eye muscle palsies electrooculography has a special significance especially in connection with the application of Edrophonium, which normalizes myasthenic hypometric saccades and transforms them even in hypermetric saccades.
  • (5) during the cover test, the keeping of fixation exhibits sometimes an hypermetric movement of the eye, which is accounted with a false localization of images.
  • (6) After Tensilon injection hypermetric saccades (overshoots) were observed which depended on a disproportion of the supranuclear oculomotor centers and the eye muscles.
  • (7) Our patients also presented the classical hypermetric misreaching when attempting to point by hand at visual targets in an otherwise dark room.
  • (8) Movements with oscillations reached the target with increased variability of end position, whereas movements without oscillations were often hypermetric.
  • (9) Lesions of the lateral cerebellum, on the contrary, were followed by inability to control accurately the amplitude of steps which were performed with a constant hypermetric error; the maintenance phase of movement was not disturbed in this group.
  • (10) Direct current electro-oculography revealed abduction nystagmus with hypermetric abduction saccades in 35 of 64 patients with unilateral and 55 of 66 patients with bilateral internuclear ophthalmoplegia.
  • (11) A captive adult puma developed ataxia, a hypermetric gait and whole body tremor.
  • (12) Superposition of impaired medial rectus inhibition and increased phasic innervation best explains abduction nystagmus with slowed hypermetric (6 unilateral and 23 bilateral cases) or normometric abduction saccades (9 unilateral and 5 bilateral cases).
  • (13) In some myasthenic patients, small amplitude saccades were hypermetric and had high velocities, appearing clinically as "quiver" movements characteristic of MG.
  • (14) Over-shooting oscillations or slightly hypermetric voluntary saccades occurred in 5 patients.
  • (15) Two dogs that had been given phenytoin for control of seizures for approximately 1 year developed signs of phenytoin toxicosis (postural ataxia an d a hypermetric gait) when chloramphenicol was added to the therapeutic regimen.
  • (16) Additional arguments for damage of cerebellar oculomotor functions are the predominance of cogwheeled smooth pursuit and the occasional observation of hypermetric saccades, both toward the side of the tumor.
  • (17) The adjustment of the amplitude of artificially induced hypermetric saccades, called gain adaptivity, was examined experimentally by using double target steps.
  • (18) Abduction nystagmus with hypermetric abduction saccades of normal velocity is explained by an increased phasic innervation adjusted to adduction paresis.
  • (19) Precision changes during plantar flexion movements were usually excessive, hypermetric and almost twice longer than preimmersion.
  • (20) Clinical examination of saccades revealed about half of the dissociated and half of the conjugate hypermetric disorders.

Syllable


Definition:

  • (n.) An elementary sound, or a combination of elementary sounds, uttered together, or with a single effort or impulse of the voice, and constituting a word or a part of a word. In other terms, it is a vowel or a diphtong, either by itself or flanked by one or more consonants, the whole produced by a single impulse or utterance. One of the liquids, l, m, n, may fill the place of a vowel in a syllable. Adjoining syllables in a word or phrase need not to be marked off by a pause, but only by such an abatement and renewal, or reenforcement, of the stress as to give the feeling of separate impulses. See Guide to Pronunciation, /275.
  • (n.) In writing and printing, a part of a word, separated from the rest, and capable of being pronounced by a single impulse of the voice. It may or may not correspond to a syllable in the spoken language.
  • (n.) A small part of a sentence or discourse; anything concise or short; a particle.
  • (v. t.) To pronounce the syllables of; to utter; to articulate.

Example Sentences:

  • (1) Real ear CVRs, calculated from real ear recordings of nonsense syllables, were obtained from eight hearing-impaired listeners.
  • (2) In addition, they were tested with dichotic listening for correct reports of consonant-vowel syllables.
  • (3) There is recent evidence that children naturally divide syllables into the opening consonant or consonant cluster (the onset) and the rest of the syllable (the rime).
  • (4) Children in the first group were provided training by their parents that was intended to focus the child's attention on consonants in syllables or words and to teach discrimination between correctly and incorrectly articulated consonants.
  • (5) Older hearing controls (14-16 years) matched the deaf group in span and tended to recall most accurately written syllables which are not easily lipread.
  • (6) Free recall of nonsense syllables was significantly better when these were learned under active compound.
  • (7) Under some conditions, visual information can override auditory information to the extent that identification judgments of a visually influenced syllable can be as consistent as for an analogous audiovisually compatible syllable.
  • (8) The major findings were as follows: (1) no significant difference was found in consonant identification scores between aperiodic, aperiodic + vocalic transition, and vocalic transition segments in CV syllables compared to those in VC syllables; (2) consonant identifications from vocalic transition + vowel segments in VC syllables were significantly greater than those from vocalic transition + vowel segments in CV syllables; (3) no significant difference was found in vowel identification scores between aperiodic + vocalic transition, vocalic transition + vowel, and vocalic transition segments in CV syllables compared to those in VC syllables; and (4) vowel identifications from aperiodic segments were significantly greater in CV syllables than in VC syllables.
  • (9) In the first, span and free-recall measures were obtained for 24 subjects, each tested with four types of spoken material (nonsense syllables, random words, fourth-order approximations to English, and normal prose).
  • (10) A reading battery composed of eight different subtests was given to each patient (reading of letters, reading of syllables, reading of pseudowords, reading of words, reading of sentences, understanding commands, reading and comprehension of texts, and logographic reading).
  • (11) "I'm Ms Dy-na-mi-TEE-ee," she sang on the chorus, putting an emphasis on the penultimate syllable.
  • (12) Using tonal stimuli based on the nonspeech stimuli of Mattingly et al., we found that subjects, with appropriate practice, could classify nonspeech chirp, short bleat, and bleat continua with boundaries equivalent to the syllable place continuum of Mattingly et al.
  • (13) After learning to categorize syllables consisting of [d], [b], or [g] followed by four different vowels, quail correctly categorized syllables in which the same consonants preceded eight novel vowels.
  • (14) Discourse passages and consonant nonsense syllables, presented in quiet and in noise, were used as the test conditions.
  • (15) The interactive effects of these modifications were evaluated by obtaining indices of nonsense syllable recognition ability from normally hearing listeners for systematically varied combinations of the four signal parameters.
  • (16) This study was designed to investigate the effects of self-evaluative responses with feedback in a nonsense syllable recognition task (Experiment I) and a concept learning task (Experiment II).
  • (17) All subjects received 60 monaural and dichotic consonant-vowel (CV) nonsense syllables presented at equal loudness levels using the most comfortable level (MCL) as the loudness criteria.
  • (18) Stutterers react emotionally to syllables they stutter because they experience difficulty in articulating those syllables.
  • (19) For the reverberant condition, the sentences were played through a room with a reverberation time of 1.2 s. The CVC syllables were removed from the sentences and presented in pairs to ten subjects with audiometrically normal hearing, who judged the similarity of the syllable pairs separately for the nonreverberant and reverberant conditions.
  • (20) Well-formed syllable production is established in the first 10 months of life by hearing infants but not by deaf infants, indicating that audition plays an important role in vocal development.

Words possibly related to "hypermetric"